August 14, 2022

12. PRIMITIVE THOUGHT | A SHORT HISTORY OF THE WORLD | H. G. WELLS

A SHORT HISTORY OF THE WORLD
BY
H. G. WELLS

12. PRIMITIVE THOUGHT

And now let us indulge in a very interesting speculation; how did it feel to be a man in those early days of the human adventure? How did men think and what did they think in those remote days of hunting and wandering four hundred centuries ago before seed time and harvest began. Those were days long before the written record of any human impressions, and we are left almost entirely to inference and guesswork in our answers to these questions.

The sources to which scientific men have gone in their attempts to

reconstruct that primitive mentality are very various. Recently the

science of psycho-analysis, which analyzes the way in which the

egotistic and passionate impulses of the child are restrained,

suppressed, modified or overlaid, to adapt them to the needs of social

life, seems to have thrown a considerable amount of light upon the

history of primitive society; and another fruitful source of suggestion

has been the study of the ideas and customs of such contemporary

savages as still survive. Again there is a sort of mental

fossilization which we find in folk-lore and the deep-lying irrational

superstitions and prejudices that still survive among modern civilized

people. And finally we have in the increasingly numerous pictures,

statues, carvings, symbols and the like, as we draw near to our own

time, clearer and clearer indications of what man found interesting and

worthy of record and representation.

Primitive man probably thought very much as a child thinks, that is to

say in a series of imaginative pictures. He conjured up images or

images presented themselves to his mind, and he acted in accordance

with the emotions they aroused. So a child or an uneducated person

does to-day. Systematic thinking is apparently a comparatively late

development in human experience; it has not played any great part in

human life until within the last three thousand years. And even to-day

those who really control and order their thoughts are but a small

minority of mankind. Most of the world still lives by imagination and

passion.

Probably the earliest human societies, in the opening stages of the

true human story, were small family groups. Just as the flocks and

herds of the earlier mammals arose out of families which remained

together and multiplied, so probably did the earliest tribes. But

before this could happen a certain restraint upon the primitive

egotisms of the individual had to be established. The fear of the

father and respect for the mother had to be extended into adult life,

and the natural jealousy of the old man of the group for the younger

males as they grew up had to be mitigated. The mother on the other

hand was the natural adviser and protector of the young. Human social

life grew up out of the reaction between the crude instinct of the

young to go off and pair by themselves as they grew up, on the one

hand, and the dangers and disadvantages of separation on the other. An

anthropological writer of great genius, J. J. Atkinson, in his _Primal

Law_, has shown how much of the customary law of savages, the _Tabus_,

that are so remarkable a fact in tribal life, can be ascribed to such a

mental adjustment of the needs of the primitive human animal to a

developing social life, and the later work of the psycho- analysts has

done much to confirm his interpretation of these possibilities.

Some speculative writers would have us believe that respect and fear of

the Old Man and the emotional reaction of the primitive savage to older

protective women, exaggerated in dreams and enriched by fanciful mental

play, played a large part in the beginnings of primitive religion and

in the conception of gods and goddesses. Associated with this respect

for powerful or helpful personalities was a dread and exaltation of

such personages after their deaths, due to their reappearance in

dreams. It was easy to believe they were not truly dead but only

fantastically transferred to a remoteness of greater power.


The dreams, imaginations and fears of a child are far more vivid and

real than those of a modern adult, and primitive man was always

something of a child. He was nearer to the animals also, and he could

suppose them to have motives and reactions like his own. He could

imagine animal helpers, animal enemies, animal gods. One needs to have

been an imaginative child oneself to realize again how important,

significant, portentous or friendly, strangely shaped rocks, lumps of

wood, exceptional trees or the like may have appeared to the men of the

Old Stone Age, and how dream and fancy would create stories and legends

about such things that would become credible as they told them. Some

of these stories would be good enough to remember and tell again. The

women would tell them to the children and so establish a tradition. To

this day most imaginative children invent long stories in which some

favourite doll or animal or some fantastic semi-human being figures as

the hero, and primitive man probably did the same—with a much stronger

disposition to believe his hero real.


For the very earliest of the true men that we know of were probably

quite talkative beings. In that way they have differed from the

Neanderthalers and had an advantage over them. The Neanderthaler may

have been a dumb animal. Of course the primitive human speech was

probably a very scanty collection of names, and may have been eked out

with gestures and signs.

There is no sort of savage so low as not to have a kind of science of

cause and effect. But primitive man was not very critical in his

associations of cause with effect; he very easily connected an effect

with something quite wrong as its cause. “You do so and so,” he said,

“and so and so happens.” You give a child a poisonous berry and it

dies. You eat the heart of a valiant enemy and you become strong.

There we have two bits of cause and effect association, one true one

false. We call the system of cause and effect in the mind of a savage,

Fetish; but Fetish is simply savage science. It differs from modern

science in that it is totally unsystematic and uncritical and so more

frequently wrong.

In many cases it is not difficult to link cause and effect, in many

others erroneous ideas were soon corrected by experience; but there was

a large series of issues of very great importance to primitive man,

where he sought persistently for causes and found explanations that

were wrong but not sufficiently wrong nor so obviously wrong as to be

detected. It was a matter of great importance to him that game should

be abundant or fish plentiful and easily caught, and no doubt he tried

and believed in a thousand charms, incantations and omens to determine

these desirable results. Another great concern of his was illness and

death. Occasionally infections crept through the land and men died of

them. Occasionally men were stricken by illness and died or were

enfeebled without any manifest cause. This too must have given the

hasty, emotional mind of primitive man much feverish exercise. Dreams

and fantastic guesses made him blame this, or appeal for help to that

man or beast or thing. He had the child’s aptitude for fear and panic.

Quite early in the little human tribe, older, steadier minds sharing

the fears, sharing the imaginations, but a little more forceful than

the others, must have asserted themselves, to advise, to prescribe, to

command. This they declared unpropitious and that imperative, this an

omen of good and that an omen of evil. The expert in Fetish, the

Medicine Man, was the first priest. He exhorted, he interpreted

dreams, he warned, he performed the complicated hocus pocus that

brought luck or averted calamity. Primitive religion was not so much

what we now call religion as practice and observance, and the early

priest dictated what was indeed an arbitrary primitive practical

science.